19 November 2014

Notes - this is what we do / quasar alignment / grandma story / senses and definitions

         Mid-morning. The snow has blown off the tree and bush branches on this cloudy windy morning. You are feeling a bit more ‘grounded’ today, mostly with the recognition that knowing little to next to nothing gives you a sense of bliss that allows you to refocus on what you enjoy writing about the human condition which might as well begin as a seed rocked by an Angel somewhat akin to the natural environment of this particular universe. – Amorella

         0937 hours. I would not think that I would make such a consideration but it does sound as reasonable as any other. We begin as next to nothing and we conclude nearly the same, so why should the middle be any different save for ego and theatre. We have the ability to make noise so this is what we do.

         Post. - Amorella


         1056 hours. Here is something new:
*****
19-Nov-2014

Spooky alignment of quasars across billions of light-years
VLT reveals alignments

***
Artist's Impression of Mysterious Alignment of Quasar Rotation Axes


Caption: This artist's impression shows schematically the mysterious alignments between the spin axes of quasars and the large-scale structures that they inhabit that observations with ESO's Very Large Telescope have revealed. These alignments are over billions of light-years and are the largest known in the universe.

The large-scale structure is shown in blue and quasars are marked in white with the rotation axes of their black holes indicated with a line.

This picture is for illustration only and does not depict the real distribution of galaxies and quasars.

Credit: ESO/M. Kornmesser
Usage Restrictions: None

***
Quasars are galaxies with very active supermassive black holes at their centres. These black holes are surrounded by spinning discs of extremely hot material that is often spewed out in long jets along their axes of rotation. Quasars can shine more brightly than all the stars in the rest of their host galaxies put together.

A team led by Damien Hutsemékers from the University of Liège in Belgium used the FORS instrument on the VLT to study 93 quasars that were known to form huge groupings spread over billions of light-years, seen at a time when the Universe was about one third of its current age.

"The first odd thing we noticed was that some of the quasars' rotation axes were aligned with each other -- despite the fact that these quasars are separated by billions of light-years," said Hutsemékers.

The team then went further and looked to see if the rotation axes were linked, not just to each other, but also to the structure of the Universe on large scales at that time.

When astronomers look at the distribution of galaxies on scales of billions of light-years they find that they are not evenly distributed. They form a cosmic web of filaments and clumps around huge voids where galaxies are scarce. This intriguing and beautiful arrangement of material is known as large-scale structure.

The new VLT results indicate that the rotation axes of the quasars tend to be parallel to the large-scale structures in which they find themselves. So, if the quasars are in a long filament then the spins of the central black holes will point along the filament. The researchers estimate that the probability that these alignments are simply the result of chance is less than 1%.

"A correlation between the orientation of quasars and the structurethey belong to is an important prediction of numerical models of evolution of our Universe. Our data provide the first observational confirmation of this effect, on scales much larger that what had been observed to date for normal galaxies," adds Dominique Sluse of the Argelander-Institut für Astronomie in Bonn, Germany and University of Liège.

The team could not see the rotation axes or the jets of the quasars directly. Instead they measured the polarisation of the light from each quasar and, for 19 of them, found a significantly polarised signal. The direction of this polarisation, combined with other information, could be used to deduce the angle of the accretion disc and hence the direction of the spin axis of the quasar.

"The alignments in the new data, on scales even bigger than current predictions from simulations, may be a hint that there is a missing ingredient in our current models of the cosmos," concludes Dominique Sluse.

###
More information

This research was presented in a paper entitled "Alignment of quasar polarizations with large-scale structures", by D. Hutsemékers et al., to appear in the journal Astronomy & Astrophysics on 19 November 2014.

The team is composed of D. Hutsemékers (Institut d'Astrophysique et de Géophysique, Université de Liège, Liège, Belgium), L. Braibant (Liège), V. Pelgrims (Liège) and D. Sluse (Argelander-Institut für Astronomie, Bonn, Germany; Liège).

ESO is the foremost intergovernmental astronomy organisation in Europe and the world's most productive ground-based astronomical observatory by far. It is supported by 15 countries: Austria, Belgium, Brazil, the Czech Republic, Denmark, France, Finland, Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Portugal, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland and the United Kingdom. ESO carries out an ambitious programme focused on the design, construction and operation of powerful ground-based observing facilities enabling astronomers to make important scientific discoveries. ESO also plays a leading role in promoting and organising cooperation in astronomical research. ESO operates three unique world-class observing sites in Chile: La Silla, Paranal and Chajnantor. At Paranal, ESO operates the Very Large Telescope, the world's most advanced visible-light astronomical observatory and two survey telescopes. VISTA works in the infrared and is the world's largest survey telescope and the VLT Survey Telescope is the largest telescope designed to exclusively survey the skies in visible light. ESO is the European partner of a revolutionary astronomical telescope ALMA, the largest astronomical project in existence. ESO is currently planning the 39-metre European Extremely Large optical/near-infrared Telescope, the E-ELT, which will become "the world's biggest eye on the sky".
From - http://www.eurekalertDOTorg/
*****
         You almost automatically sent Doug a copy of this article. The point, to you, is that an organization exists within physics that is very broad and yet almost invisible to human observation. This, to you, shows there may be much more to reality than humans suspect. You would like to hope so to give humans a greater grasp of humility than they appear to show. – Amorella

         1112 hours. I did not consider the conclusion of your statement; but it is reasonable to me. It is like I am standing in the classroom pointed to the chalkboard and saying, “See, this is what we do not know.” I will not apologize for this.

         Good. Post. - Amorella


         After noon. You have been working on the Scottish and English genealogies in “Grandma’s Story” segments leading up to “The Brothers” segments and can better see the problems as you ran off two name/date sheets and can compare the families directly across from one another. – Amorella

         1223 hours. The main problems stem from the ninth century but I am correcting them. Too many names, too many dates that have to better correlate. This is a tedious task but certainly not insurmountable. It reminds me so much of real family genealogies. People are missing and the numbers show it, either than or the numbers are wrong – a different person or a different generation with the same name or three or five generations with the same first name. I am going to keep one name only up into the fifteenth century, that is a century before the Tudor’s. Then I have to have names that are similar but corruptible to the modern last names of Greystone and Bleacher.

         You had an earlier than usual supper at Panera on Mason-Montgomery while running errands after Carol had finished at the Hair Dresser’s. Once home you watched several programs but called it quits before nine. Tomorrow the furnace people will be here at eight for a ‘clean up’ for winter inspection, etc. You were ready to shut the computer down when you checked a BBC article that appears interesting. Drop it in and that’s all for tonight. Post. – Amorella

** **
BBC Future - IN DEPTH
 19 November 2014

Psychology: How many senses do we have?
By Christian Jarrett

We often talk of having five senses as a universal truth. In reality, there may be more – or fewer – depending on the way you look at the question. Christian Jarrett explains the controversy.

Some myths about the brain, such as the idea we only use 10% of our grey matter, are notorious, especially among neuroscientists. These myths crop up every now and then (look at the premise of the Lucy movie this summer), but they are quickly shot down by those in the know.

In contrast to these enduring stories, other misconceptions are stealthier and slip beneath the radar unrecognised. One of these is the idea that the human brain is served by five senses. This belief is so ingrained that even the scientifically literate will treat it as taken-for-granted common knowledge.

Perhaps it is due to the idea’s noble origins. The principle of five basic human senses is often traced back to Aristotle’s De Anima (On the Soul), in which he devotes a separate chapter to vision, hearing, touch, smell and taste. Today, the five senses are considered such an elementary truth that it is sometimes used as a point of consensus before writers embark on more mysterious or contentious topics. “What do we actually mean by reality?” asked the author of a recent article in New Scientist magazine. “A straightforward answer is that it means everything that appears to our five senses.”

Incoming information

If only it were that simple. Simply defining what we mean by a “sense” leads you down a slippery slope into philosophy. One, somewhat vague, definition might argue that a human sense is simply a unique way for the brain to receive information about the world and the body. If that is the case, then we can claim with confidence that there are certainly more than five human senses.

First consider the senses that relate to the position of our bodies. Close your eyes, and then touch your right forefinger to your left elbow tip. Easy? How did you do it? Somehow you knew where the end of your finger was and you also knew the position of your left elbow. This sense is known as proprioception and it’s the awareness we have of where each of our body parts is located in space. Proprioception is possible thanks to receptors in our muscles known as spindles, which tell the brain about the current length and stretch of the muscles.
Now imagine you are blindfolded and I tilted you forwards slowly. You’d immediately have a sensation of how your body’s position was changing in relation to gravity. This is thanks to the fluid-filled vestibular system in your inner ear, which helps us keep balance. This system also gives us our experience of acceleration through space, and it links up with the eyes, making it possible to cancel out our own motion. If you wiggle your head around while reading, for example, you’ll see that it makes little difference to your ability to read and stay focused on the words.

There are also numerous senses providing us with information about the inner state of our bodies. The most obvious of these are hunger and thirst, inner body pain, and the need to empty the bladder or bowel. Less obvious and less available to conscious awareness are incoming signals about blood pressure, the pH level of the cerebrospinal fluid, plus many more.

Some might take that definition further, to argue that the senses should be defined by the types of receptors we have; a different sensor means a different sense. If that were the case, then even well-known senses quickly split into different varieties. For instance, if you closed your eyes and I surprised you with an ice cube down your back, you’d experience a shock of cold. This sensation would be distinct from the mere touch of a plastic cube, say. Alongside temperature-sensitive receptors, packed in our skin we also have receptors dedicated to mechanical pressure, pain (known as nociceptors) and itch (pruritic receptors). Using the same logic, however, taste can be divided into sweet, sour, salty and bitter and potentially “umami”, which is activated by monosodium glutamate and is associated with a “meaty” flavour. Splitting the senses in this way doesn’t feel like the most intuitive way of dealing with the question, however, and it becomes even more absurd if we turn to smell: humans have over 1,000 distinct olfactory receptors tuned to different odorous molecules. Should each one be counted as a different sense?

At the other extreme, you could restrict our definition of discrete sense to the physical categories of incoming information. We can simplify the human senses down to just three – mechanical (which takes in touch, hearing and proprioception); chemical (including taste, smell and internal senses); and light.

Yet another way of approaching this issue is to think not about the category of incoming information or the perceptual experience, but about how incoming sensory information is used. A great example is the human capacity for echo-location. Human echo-location works by a person emitting a clicking sound with the tongue and listening for how it rebounds off the immediate environment. In the USA there is even a remarkable team of blind cyclists – Team Bat – led by Daniel Kisch, who use echo-location to go mountain biking (see www.worldaccessfortheblind.org for videos). This ability depends on the traditional sense of hearing, but the perceptual experience and function is more akin to vision. You don’t need to be blind to try it; even sighted people can learn to “see in the dark” using echo-location. For these reasons, some consider it a separate sense.

As you can see, there is no single, logical way to define the senses. In some ways, it might make little sense to draw divisions between them at all – considering that they often seem to blend together; the colour of food – and even the sounds of a restaurant – can influence taste, for instance. Understanding these relationships is important when studying conditions like synaesthesia and could even shed light on consciousness itself.

But whichever way you look at it, five is a pretty arbitrary and meaningless number – a glaring “myth” of the brain that needs further recognition. Indeed, once you start thinking about all the different kinds of information reaching the human brain, you might even find that you develop a brand new sense – a radar-like sensitivity to some of the other misconceptions regarding the way the brain experiences the world. You might have once called it a “sixth sense” – but you know better now, don’t you?

This article is based on a chapter from Christian Jarrett’s new book Great Myths of the Brain (Wiley).

From BBC Future, 19 November 2014
** **

         2204 hours. This is interesting and I understand the point in terms of definition of the senses. It is down to definition first. I don’t believe I have any other than the five usual senses. What is a sixth sense anyway?

** **

English definition of “sixth sense”

Noun – an ability to know something without using the ordinary five senses of sight, hearing, smell, touch and taste –

[My sixth sense told me something awful was going to happen.]

(Definition of sixth sense from the Cambridge Academic Content Dictionary; Cambridge University Press)

** **
         2213 hours. As long as ‘sixth sense’ is used as a synonym for ‘intuition’ I will accept it in context.

** **

intuition – noun

the ability to understand something immediately, without the need for conscious reasoning: we shall allow our intuition to guide us.

• a thing that one knows or considers likely from instinctive feeling rather than conscious reasoning: your insights and intuitions as a native speaker are positively sought.

ORIGIN late Middle English (denoting spiritual insight or immediate spiritual communication): from late Latin intuitio(n-), from Latin intueri ‘consider’

Selected and edited from the Oxford/American software

** **

         2217 hours. I stand by the Oxford/American Dictionary definition of ‘intuition’. I do from time to time have what I feel is ‘intuition’. I use it in the direction of my reasoning. It is insight but I do not know that it is or is not spiritual. I must say here, I feel that my intuition has a spiritual orientation, but there is no proof this is spiritual, and I would not argue with anyone who would say that it is not spiritual. No doubt there are some examples of my intuition put to use within the confines of this several years of published blog. Examples are not proof though, not in my mind. 


No comments:

Post a Comment