30 October 2015

Notes - 29/30 October

29 October 2015

         Thursday morning. You are readying to head to The Columbia in the Ybor City Historical District (Tampa) for lunch. You did your forty minutes of exercises this morning and feel better for it. – Amorella

         1036 hours. I am having the 1905 salad for lunch. It has been at least a decade since I order it. Usually I have a different combination of chicken and rice.

30 October 2015

         The four of you went to the AMC at West Shore Shopping late morning. Carol and Linda saw Our Brand in Crisis and you and Bill saw Steve Jobs then a stop at the nearby Panera for a light lunch before heading home. Lunch discussion was on the films; the girls liked the overall political focus/statement in Our Brand and you both enjoyed the focus and drama in the Jobs’ film. – Amorella

         2013 hours. I was surprised at the Shakespeare-like character presentations in Steve Jobs. I teared up a couple times – the emotion was unexpected – but then much of the focus was on Steve Jobs, his closest friend Steve Wozniak, his daughter, Lisa Brennan-Jobs and John Sculley. Steve Jobs was portrayed and played well as a tragic Shakespearean character. I read an online interview with Steve Wozniak who says the film does a very good job of showing aspects of the real Steve Jobs he knew in his adult life.

         After lunch you relaxed at Linda and Bill’s, spending time with Carol and Linda in the hot tub while Bill worked on his backyard gardening. Tomorrow, early afternoon, you and Carol are to the condo for a week. Linda is planning on staying over Sunday night. – Post. – Amorella

         2042 hours. I am tired, thought I would take a nap earlier but at five we were out to an early supper to meet with Jean, assistant principal, who has duty at the Blake High School football game tonight. Tomorrow she is off to see her husband at an air force base in southern Georgia; returning Sunday afternoon. 

27 October 2015

Notes - trip and folios /

         You were out the door a few minutes late but it made no difference. There was a southbound accident just south of I-71 and Red Bank Road. Traffic was stopped before Fields-Ertel Road. It took you from 0710 to 0835 to cross the Ohio River. Otherwise, no problems until south of Atlanta at 1640 hours when traffic crept until 1720 or so when you got off at Exit 218. You are staying at the Holiday Inn Express and ate at the nearby Cracker Barrel. Navigation said it was going to be stop and go traffic for the next twenty miles and further down at Exit 138 there was a major accident southbound. – Amorella

         2006 hours. There was no question what to do. We stopped for the night. It rained for all but about ten minutes of the drive. The motel is pleasant enough but the Internet is not connecting to the MacAir.

         You spent time reading the directions to your Character Folio. The suggestion is to have one character folio for each of the four selections. However, I, the Amorella, feel it would be better to have a Basic Body Folio: Merlyn, The Supervisor and Grandma and the Dead; Underwriting Folio: Merlyn, you and me, the Amorella; The Brothers Folio: Robert and Richard; The Grandma Folio: Grandma and the Dead; The Pouch Folio A: Friendly, Yermey, Hartolite. Pouch Folio B: Blake, Pyl and Justin. Pouch Folio C: Pyl and Yermey; Friendly and Blake; Hartolite and Justin. And, last but not least – You and each of the characters separately.

         2114 hours. What about you and me?

         Not in your lifetime, boy. Post. – Amorella

         2115 hours. This is too much work.

         Just work up the different folios for now. - Amorella

26 October 2015

Notes - readying / new software

         Mid-afternoon. This morning you voted early in the Ohio general election. You had a late lunch at Panera/Chipotle where you chatted with Normb your one time Kroger butcher (now retired) working part time at Panera; Carol is presently picking up cat food at Kroger’s on Tylersville. Next, to the bank then you are on to Kroger’s on Mason-Montgomery for freshly baked cookies for tomorrow’s trip. Once home you will continue packing. You are hopeful the weather won’t be too bad tomorrow traffic-wise. – Amorella

         1439 hours. I will drive professionally (defensively and with anticipation) as always on I-75. It is a good drive both ways. I have been driving I-75 for 43 years – I pretty much know what to look for on any given stretch – I try to average 75 (depending on conditions and traffic) once out of urban areas – rarely over 80 or 90 except to pass or to get out of someone’s road.

         You are home and have had your ‘talk’ with Jadah. The key words are in a more serious but polite tone – “Jadah, you and Spooky have to take care of the house and each other. Amy will be over to take care of you. We will be back later. You and Spooky have to take care of the house.” - Amorella

         1548 hours. I am always holding her when I give the talk. It seems to put her to ease. She ‘knows’ something’s up when; 1, the suitcases are brought out, and; 2, clothes are being brought out to be hung and/or sorted.

         Post. Time to get packing, boy. – Amorella

         Late afternoon. You are mostly packed. You need to take two sweatshirts and a light coat. – You were looking through Apple apps and found an interesting one called “Character Folio”. – Amorella

         1717 hours. This app looks interesting but I am not sure of its use this late in the game. Some reviews say it is a bit expensive but most say it is a very good app for writers.

         2224 hours. The Character Folio is for holding/building background on a character. It looks useful and is rated well.


         You are opening up a new software dimension. Go with the Folio if you wish. Have a safe trip, orndorff. – Post. Amorella

25 October 2015

Notes - work day Sun day /

         You and Carol spent the morning and the first part of the afternoon raking and cleaning up the yard and putting the outdoor furniture away for Winter. You had a late lunch at Penn Station and ice cream at Graeter’s. Carol worked on three loads of laundry during the day also and you cleaned and fueled the Avalon for Tuesday’s trip to somewhere below Atlanta and perhaps Macon, Georgia depending the rainy weather caused by last week’s Pacific hurricane. The recycle trash and garbage as well as seven large bags of leaves are out the morning pickup. Carol is watching “The Good Wife”. Earlier, during supper, you watched a “Modern Family” and a “Home Fires” along with tonight’s “Madam Secretary”. Such was your Sunday. – Amorella

         2147 hours. I had an hour nap while Carol was working on laundry. I walked 3.2 miles today and I’m sure Carol walked another two beyond that, and we didn’t even get to the park. Early morning I did check my new external hard drive and it fully recorded my MacAir through Apple’s Time Machine app Saturday night. I am fully confident in both the hardware and the software. The MacAir is efficiently lean and clean. 

            Post. - Amorella         

24 October 2015

Notes - the boys /

         Shortly after noon. You are at Pine Hill Lakes Park – Owen and Brennan are in the kids’ area after feeding the ducks and geese. Carol and Kim made breakfast – sausage links and pancakes and a fruit cup. You are going to Outback for lunch then they are leaving from there. – Amorella

         You had lunch at Outback, Paul’s treat. You and Carol tried the tacos (steak and chicken) and decided lunch is much better at Longhorn. They are on their way home and you are both happy they stayed as long as they did. You both noted how Owen and Brennan are actually having conversations  (reasonable dialogues) and getting along much better as brothers. This you are quite pleased to see. Paul and Kim said the best surprise of the trip was how well both boys behaved and adapted to the environment at hand. – Amorella

         1425 hours. It is surprising to see how much better they get along. I mean, they are boys who are brothers but they are not squabbling as much as they were and seem, most of the time, to enjoy each other’s company. Brennan in particular is much, much better than he was even three months ago. It appears the long stretched ‘terrible two’s’ are finally over.

         Post. - Amorella


23 October 2015

Notes - "Tradition!" / needle and thread / unconsciousness to consciousness

         Noon. You called Kim; they are at McD’s at Calhoun, Kentucky and will be here before five, then out to supper at Olive Garden. You got your haircut this morning, so that’s another chore off the list. Larry Meiber’s is stopping by this afternoon to pick up the check for the condo; the energy is so building for leaving on Tuesday. Carol has the grocery to do and probably Smashburgers for lunch. The beds are ready for visitors; and it’s another beautiful Fall day. – Amorella

         1207 hours. I thought for sure Kim would say they would stop by for supper and then drive on home tonight. I think they still might – nothing like sleeping in your own bed. Then again, the beds here, the twins (once bunks) and the queen were all Kim’s beds at one time or another (newer mattresses though). The twin beds were Carol and Mary Lou’s when they were in Korea and Vietnam as well as when in Alexandria, Va. I know she misses Mary Lou. We all do. Kim is so happy to have inherited her dining room table, chairs and china cabinet and looks forward to continuing the family traditions with it.

         Post. - Amorella

        You had lunch at Smashburgers then stopped at the bank, and are presently in the Kroger’s lot on Mason-Montgomery Road. Kim and Paul will be at Berea, Kentucky within the half hour. Anticipation is growing. – Amorella

         1349 hours. We had a good lunch and our chores are almost complete. It is about two hours from Berea to Mason and Kim thought they would arrive as early as four. Hopefully the boys got played out at McDonalds and are napping, at least Brennan. Kim said they just finished watching the film, Frozen. At least they have something to do, though into elementary school and beyond Kim always took books to read, mostly novels – she and Carol both. I sure hope they like to read as well as the other entertainments.

         This statement is from a fellow who never reads for fun. – Amorella

         1355 hours. I know. Reading is so important. Reading is paved with grammar and logic is the foundation of grammar. The other entertainments do not drive so deep into the human brain/thought. Grammar is my keel, no question about it.

         How will this grammar business work for the Dead? Where is reason’s basic path? – Amorella

         1401 hours. Metaphysical stuffing? Reasoning is needed for survival, but the Dead survive anyway, at least in here, in the Merlyn novels. If the Dead have a focus, a North Star so to speak, then that is the focus – to get from where they are to someplace else.

         That is already set up. – Amorella

         1407 hours. The individual and the community, the needle and thread, are needed to sew the species together along the way. 

         What purpose is the marsupial humanoid in this line of thought? – Amorella

         1411 hours. I don’t know. Interesting. We have to have something that connects to two species ‘spirits’ – a reason, a need, for the two species to connect. Since Diplomat is/will be a hybrid of Pyl and Yermey, then she brings the DNA of the two species together and there is then a ‘hybrid’ (heartansoulanmind) spirit that further ‘evolves’ or ‘gains-a-momentum’ or ‘becomes’. The ‘glue’ is when the spirit embeds into the DNA. (1417)

         The boys are too wired to sleep at the moment. Graeter’s ice cream didn’t help. You had supper at Olive Garden and had a good chat too. You also stopped at Best Buy and bought a Seagate Slim for Mac 1 TB External USB 3.0/2.0 Portable Hard Drive with a Mobile Backup App./with Time Machine – data transfer rates up to 4.8 Gbps. Then you all went to Graeter’s. Once home Paul fixed the TV sound to work  with the sound bar automatically and showed you how the simulated 3-D worked with the glasses. You had never checked and were surprised how good it was. – Amorella

         2122 hours. I cannot believe we have had that TV for more than a year and four months and never checked it out. Paul also fixed the Internet service and Netflix to work better than they did, that is, he made them work more consistent and automatic. This took him about twenty minutes total. Amazing. We are indeed old. I remember helping Mom and Dad Hammond set up and work their VCR. Every time we went down to their house in Florida the set time was off, and blinking instead. Now I know how they felt. It reminds me of those classic Seinfeld episodes where he visits his parents in Florida and they fight over where to set the air conditioning temperature. Sometimes his parents didn’t even seem to know how their air conditioner/heater thermostat worked. Ah, such is modern life.

         You are more thankful of your life and the people in it than you think, boy. – Post.

         2132 hours. Strange, but I presently understand this. You know my heart feelings before they rise to consciousness. I don’t know how, but you do. It is as though you are a seemingly unnatural though completely natural embodiment of (my) unconsciousness, an expeditor of consciousness from (my) unconscious realm. This is not intuition. It is experienced; it is a reality within myself. Surely all these notes and Merlyn books show something is about within myself.

         You are exceedingly joyous that you have your notes from way back into the early eighties. You feel others have this ability to sense unconsciousness consciously but you do not know how to direct people to attempt the experiences for themselves. – Amorella

         2147 hours. I am even glad I put this online so I am free of it at the same time. Let someone else wonder about the ‘inner workings’ of unconscious thought to conscious thought. Maybe there is something to it. In my lifetime they have shown that plants have a sort of consciousness; maybe some day they will show people have a greater ‘fuller’ sense of consciousness and unconsciousness than we consider we have.


         Paul just appeared from the TV room and said he was going to bed (where everyone else is). Post, orndorff, and get some sleep. - Amorella

22 October 2015

Notes - seemingly a busy day /

         Mid-morning. The house is ‘cleaned up’ for Jill the cleaning lady. Carol is at breakfast with her friends, you are relaxing. – Amorella

         Shortly after noon local time. You are sitting at McD’s near Kings Island watching the cars go by after your walk at Pine Hill Lakes Park. Carol is reading the morning paper and you both cold dollar drinks. - Amorella

         1224 hours. If I had been thinking I would have brought McD coupons and we could have had two breakfast egg macmuffins for lunch.

         You have developed a full-fledged cold. Very exciting. – Amorella

         1231 hours. I have worked on this for a couple of weeks. Glad it’s finally here, perhaps it will be gone by the time we head to Florida.

         That’s only five days, boy. – Amorella

         1232 hours. Okay, maybe it will be gone by the time we return. I’m ready for a nap.

         Later. You drove to Potbelly’s on Mason-Montgomery for a take out regular chicken salad on wheat and a barbeque chip drove to the nearby McD’s for a shaded spot to eat, then a stop at Graeter’s on Mason-Montgomery before a return to the park for reading at the far north lot. While you were both stretching your legs from too much sitting you noticed the iPhone has Kim, Paul and the boys heading north on the Turnpike from Orlando to I-75 north. You called to confirm. Kim said she was going to call once they were on I-75. They left a night early having seen everything at least once. Their money was refunded and the saw Epcot this morning; leaving at two this afternoon. They want to get to Calhoun, Georgia tonight (north of Atlanta) and are stopping by tomorrow for supper out and an overnight, leaving in the morning for home. – Amorella

         1532 hours. We have been home for about forty minutes. The house is clean, so good timing on their arrival. Carol is checking out the bedrooms for sheets, etc. We are excited they are stopping tomorrow evening; always good to see them. I’m sure they’re tired but getting through Atlanta about ten tonight is the way to go and they’ll be through Cincinnati about four, before rush hour.

         Bedtime. You had a well-needed nap this afternoon. You both had left over Papa John’s pizza for supper and watched NBC News and “Limitless” and another you forget. About an hour ago you came up to work on a new app called “Disk Aid” and what it does is allow you to easily delete duplicate disks. In less than thirty minutes you deleted over one thousand files of duplicate material. You are amazed by the technology. – Amorella

         2254 hours. I cannot imagine how many man-hours this app saved me. Tomorrow I am going to buy a new external hard drive as the one I have is about a decade old and I will keep it as a backup for all earlier material. I presently have about 6400 files on my computer. I’m sure there are more duplicates but enough deletions for tonight. 

21 October 2015

Notes - no need to shout / WGN - Manhattan / Kafka tone

         Late morning. You returned from the park. Carol walked about two miles and you walked over one. Yesterday it was about the same at this time of day. – Amorella

         1117 hours. I took a pain pill earlier. It has been awhile. The pill helped but not much. These are the six-hour pills I have left from after the carpel tunnel operations. It is another beautiful Fall day.

         Three-fourths the way through the walk you sat on a bench overlooking the larger lake, sat quietly and thought – ‘I am the trees’. The concept was enlarging-in-the-moment. – Amorella

         1123 hours. I was not going to mention it because it was not a real transcendental moment – a real one would not have been conscious brought. I have been trees before. I gain a consciousness of a momentary multitude of roots and limbs – earth to sky, so to speak, and I lose the other particulars of humanity. Sounds silly now, but in the moment I feel closer to the natural order of how life is for much of life on the planet. Humanity rises up quickly shuffling the simplicity out of the way for complex matters. Nature is not tranquil but our species is alien here; that’s my observation. Others have their own viewpoints. I don’t think humanity makes much difference when you have roots and limbs as well as earth, sun and water. Roots and limbs grow until they don’t, it is pretty much that simple. No need to shout about it either. (1133)

         Post, with ‘no need to shout’ as the headliner. – Amorella

         1134 hours. I never know what is going to come up when I put my fingers on the keyboard. Really.

        You had Papa John’s for supper after running several errands while Carol was working on washing clothes and cleaning up as Jill K. is coming tomorrow to clean. Carol has breakfast with several of her retired Blue Ash colleagues. You watched NBC News, the first episode of the new season of “Manhattan” and “NCIS”.

         2156 hours. I love “Manhattan”. I thought of the tone of Kafka’s The Trial in tonight’s episode.

** **
The Trial
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This article is about the novel by Kafka.

The Trial (original German title: Der Process, later Der Prozess, Der Proceß and Der Prozeß) is a novel written by Franz Kafka from 1914 to 1915 and published in 1925. One of his best-known works, it tells the story of a man arrested and prosecuted by a remote, inaccessible authority, with the nature of his crime revealed neither to him nor to the reader. Heavily influenced by Dostoevsky’ Crime and Punishment and The Brothers Karamazov, Kafka even went so far as to call Dostoevsky a blood relative. Like Kafka's other novels, The Trial was never completed, although it does include a chapter, which brings the story to an end.

After Kafka's death in 1924 his friend and literary executor Max Brod edited the text for publication by Verlag Die Schmiede. The original manuscript is held at the Museum of Modern Literature, Marbach am Neckar, Germany. The first English language translation, by Willa and Edwin Muir, was published in 1937. In 1999, the book was listed in Le Monde’s 100 Books of the Century and as No. 2 of the Best German Novels of the Twentieth Century.
Plot

On his thirtieth birthday, the chief financial officer of a bank, Josef K., is unexpectedly arrested by two unidentified agents from an unspecified agency for an unspecified crime. The agents' boss later arrives and holds a mini-tribunal in the room of K.'s neighbor, Fräulein Bürstner. K. is not taken away, however, but left "free" and told to await instructions from the Committee of Affairs. He goes to work, and that night apologizes to Fräulein Bürstner for the intrusion into her room. At the end of the conversation he suddenly kisses her.

K. receives a phone call summoning him to court, and the coming Sunday is arranged as the date. No time is set, but the address is given to him. The address turns out to be a huge tenement building.
K. has to explore to find the court, which turns out to be in the attic. The room is airless, shabby, and crowded, and although he has no idea what he is charged with, or what authorizes the process, K. makes a long speech denigrating the whole process, including the agents who arrested him; during this speech an attendant's wife and a man engage in sexual activities. K. then returns home.

K. later goes to visit the court again, although he has not been summoned, and finds that it is not in session. He instead talks with the attendant's wife, who attempts to seduce him into taking her away, and who gives him more information about the process and offers to help him. K. later goes with the attendant to a higher level of the attic where the shabby and airless offices of the court are housed.

K. returns home to find Fräulein Montag, a lodger from another room, moving in with Fräulein Bürstner. He suspects that this is to prevent him from pursuing his affair with the latter woman. Yet another lodger, Captain Lanz, appears to be in league with Montag.

Later, in a store room at his own bank, K. discovers the two agents who arrested him being whipped by a flogger for asking K. for bribes and as a result of complaints K. made at court. K. tries to argue with the flogger, saying that the men need not be whipped, but the flogger cannot be swayed. The next day he returns to the storeroom and is shocked to find everything as he had found it the day before, including the whipper and the two agents.

K. is visited by his uncle, who was K.'s guardian. The uncle seems distressed by K.'s predicament. At first sympathetic, he becomes concerned that K. is underestimating the seriousness of the case. The uncle introduces K. to a lawyer, who is attended by Leni, a nurse, who K.'s uncle suspects is the advocate's mistress. During the discussion it becomes clear how different this process is from regular legal proceedings: guilt is assumed, the bureaucracy running it is vast with many levels, and everything is secret, from the charge, to the rules of the court, to the authority behind the courts – even the identity of the judges at the higher levels. The attorney tells him that he can prepare a brief for K., but since the charge is unknown and the rules are unknown, it is difficult work. It also never may be read, but is still very important. The lawyer says that his most important task is to deal with powerful court officials behind the scenes. As they talk, the lawyer reveals that the Chief Clerk of the Court has been sitting hidden in the darkness of a corner. The Chief Clerk emerges to join the conversation, but K. is called away by Leni, who takes him to the next room, where she offers to help him and seduces him. They have a sexual encounter. Afterwards K. meets his uncle outside, who is angry, claiming that K.'s lack of respect has hurt K.'s case.

K. visits the lawyer several times. The lawyer tells him incessantly how dire his situation is and tells many stories of other hopeless clients and of his behind-the-scenes efforts on behalf of these clients, and brags about his many connections. The brief is never complete. K.'s work at the bank deteriorates as he is consumed with worry about his case.

K. is surprised by one of his bank clients, who tells K. that he is aware that K. is dealing with a trial. The client learned of K.'s case from Titorelli, a painter, who has dealings with the court and told the client about K.'s case. The client advises K. to go to Titorelli for advice. Titorelli lives in the attic of a tenement in a suburb on the opposite side of town from the court that K. visited. Three teenage girls taunt K. on the steps and tease him sexually. Titorelli turns out to be an official painter of portraits for the court – an inherited position – and has a deep understanding of the process. K. learns that, to Titorelli's knowledge, not a single defendant has ever been acquitted. He sets out K.'s options and offers to help K. with either. The options are: obtain a provisional verdict of innocence from the lower court, which can be overturned at any time by higher levels of the court, which would lead to re-initiation of the process; or curry favor with the lower judges to keep the process moving at a glacial pace. Titorelli has K. leave through a small back door, as the girls are blocking the door through which K. entered. To K.'s shock, the door opens into another warren of the court's offices – again shabby and airless.

K. decides to take control of matters himself and visits his lawyer with the intention of dismissing him. At the lawyer's office he meets a downtrodden individual, Block, a client who offers K. some insight from a client's perspective. Block's case has continued for five years and he has gone from being a successful businessman to being almost bankrupt and is virtually enslaved by his dependence on the lawyer and Leni, with whom he appears to be sexually involved. The lawyer mocks Block in front of K. for his dog-like subservience. This experience further poisons K.'s opinion of his lawyer. (This chapter was left unfinished by the author.)

K. is asked by the bank to show an Italian client around local places of cultural interest, but the Italian client, short of time, asks K. to take him only to the cathedral, setting a time to meet there. When the client does not show up, K. explores the cathedral, which is empty except for an old woman and a church official. K. notices a priest who seems to be preparing to give a sermon from a small second pulpit, and K. begins to leave, lest it begin and K. be compelled to stay for its entirety. Instead of giving a sermon, the priest calls out K.'s name. K. approaches the pulpit and the priest berates him for his attitude toward the trial and for seeking help, especially from women. K. asks him to come down and the two men walk inside the cathedral. The priest works for the court as a chaplain and tells K. a fable (which was published earlier as "Before the Law") that is meant to explain his situation. K. and the priest discuss the parable. The priest tells K. that the parable is an ancient text of the court, and many generations of court officials have interpreted it differently.

On the eve of K.'s thirty-first birthday, two men arrive at his apartment. He has been waiting for them, and he offers little resistance – indeed the two men take direction from K. as they walk through town. K. leads them to a quarry where the two men place K's head on a discarded block. One of the men produces a double-edged butcher knife, and as the two men pass it back and forth between them, the narrator tells us that "K. knew then precisely, that it would have been his duty to take the knife...and thrust it into himself." He does not take the knife. One of the men holds his shoulder and pulls him up and the other man stabs him in the heart and twists the knife twice. K.'s last words are: "Like a dog!"

Selected and edited from Wikipedia

** **

         2213 hours. I remember my Uncle Ernie and Aunt Patsy telling me I had to read this book sometime during my extended college career. It was during the mid-sixties from what I remember.

         2221 hours. On another note. The Edge article arrived today. Empathy is an interesting subject. I like to think it is important in lifting our humanity. This article has other things to say about empathy too. It is usually good to see another’s point of view. If it is unsettling, that’s a good thing too because then you learn something about your own private nature. Well, that’s how I see it.  - rho

** **
From – EdgeDOTcom:

Choosing Empathy
A Conversation With Jamil Zaki

If you believe that you can harness empathy and make choices about when to experience it versus when not to, it adds a layer of responsibility to how you engage with other people. If you feel like you're powerless to control your empathy, you might be satisfied with whatever biases and limits you have on it. You might be okay with not caring about someone just because they're different from you. I want people to not feel safe empathizing in the way that they always have. I want them to understand that they're doing something deliberate when they connect with someone, and I want them to own that responsibility.

JAMIL ZAKI is an assistant professor of psychology at Stanford University and the director of the Stanford Social Neuroscience Lab.  

CHOOSING EMPATHY

I've been thinking an enormous amount about a puzzle concerning how empathy works. Before describing it, I should make sure that we're on the same page about what empathy is. To me, empathy is a useful umbrella term that captures at least three distinct but related processes through which one person responds to another person's emotions.   
  
Let's say that I run into you and you are highly distressed. A bunch of things might happen to me. One, I might "catch" your emotion and vicariously take on the same state that I see in you; that's what I would call experience sharing. Two, I might think about how you feel and why you feel the way you do. That type of explicit consideration of the world as someone else sees it is what I would call mentalizing. Three, I might develop concern for your state, and feel motivated to help you feel better; that is what people these days call compassion, also known as empathic concern.

It often seems like these processes—sharing someone's emotions, thinking about their emotions, and wanting to improve their emotional state—should always go together, but they split apart in all sorts of interesting ways. For instance, people with psychopathy are often able to understand what you feel, but feel no concern for your emotions, and thus can leverage their understanding to manipulate and even harm you.                 

I spent several years early in my career thinking about these empathic processes and how they interact with each other, but in the last couple of years, I've zoomed out. I've stopped thinking as much about the "pieces" that make up empathy and started thinking about why and when people empathize in the first place. This is where the puzzle comes in, because there are two different narratives that you might hear about how empathy works. They're both compelling and very well supported, and they're pretty much entirely contradictory with each other, at least at first blush.           
      
The first narrative is that empathy is automatic. This goes all the way back to Adam Smith, who, to me, generated the first modern account of empathy in his beautiful book, The Theory of Moral Sentiments. Smith described what he called the "fellow-feeling," through which people take on each other's states—very similar to what I would call experience sharing.                 

His most famous example of this is a crowd watching a tightrope walker. Smith said that the crowd would become nervous watching this person wobble over a precipice. Their palms would start to sweat, they'd balance and move their own bodies as though they were trying to survive the tightrope even though they were on relatively solid ground. Smith was adamant that this was something that people could not control, it just happened to them.     
            
That view dominates current theory about empathy, and not without reason. It certainly jibes with our intuition that we can't control our feeling of empathy. If I were to ask you to imagine watching someone suffer a horrendous sports injury, you probably wouldn’t think, "Well, I'd figure out how much empathy I want to feel in this moment." You'd probably predict that a wave of discomfort and empathy would just wash over you. There is lots of evidence that is what happens. People take on each other's facial expressions within a fraction of a second of seeing someone else pose an expression, even if they're not aware that they're doing it. This type of imitation happens quite early in the development. Babies, in the first weeks of their lives, will cry when they hear another infant crying. This type of sharing is probably evolutionarily old as well. Mice, who don't have the same cognitive firepower that we do, appear to take on each other's states.
My lab has been interested in another signature of empathy, which is what we call neural resonance. This is something you can capture using techniques like fMRI: When I see you experience some state—make a movement, feel pain, or exhibit some emotion—my brain generates a pattern of activity consistent with what you're experiencing, not with what I'm experiencing. It's as though my brain rehearses your experience for me so that I can understand it implicitly. We, and lots of other folks, have demonstrated that this happens, even absent any instruction to empathize and even when you distract people. This suggests that even this neural signature empathy might be occurring outside of our awareness or control.                 

That's one narrative, that empathy is automatic, and again, it’s compelling—backed by lots of evidence. But if you believe that empathy always occurs automatically, you run into a freight train of evidence to the contrary. As many of us know, there are lots of instances in which people could feel empathy, but don't. The prototype case here is intergroup settings. People who are divided by a war, or a political issue, or even a sports rivalry, often experience a collapse of their empathy. In many cases, these folks feel apathy for others on the other side of a group boundary. They fail to share, or think about, or feel concern for those other people's emotions.                 

In other cases, it gets even worse: people feel overt antipathy towards others, for instance, taking pleasure when some misfortune befalls someone on the other side of a group boundary. What's interesting to me is that this occurs not only for group boundaries that are meaningful, like ethnicity or religion, but totally arbitrary groups. If I were to divide us into a red and blue team, without that taking on any more significance, you would be more likely to experience empathy for fellow red team members than for me (apparently I'm on team blue today).                 

Another interesting feature of this group-boundedness of empathy is that it doesn't just affect the amount of empathy we feel, it also affects whether we feel empathy automatically or not. Scientists have used EEG, for instance, to demonstrate that folks exhibit less neural resonance for the pain of outgroup as compared to ingroup members, and that difference appears within 200 milliseconds. It's not that you experience automatic empathy and tamp it down if you're in an intergroup setting; it seems like in those contexts empathy doesn't occur at all.                 

You've got these two narratives. On the one hand, empathy appears automatic. On the other hand, it diminishes and expands with features of your situation. How can we square these two accounts? That's what I've been asking myself a lot these days, and I feel as though I've arrived, at least preliminarily, at an answer: we can resolve the tension between those narratives by letting go of some assumptions about how empathy works. In particular, the idea that empathy is out of our control.

Lately, I've begun thinking about empathy not as something that happens to us, but rather as a choice that we make, even if we're not aware we're making it. We often make an implicit or explicit decision as to whether we want to engage with someone's emotions or not, based on the motives we might have for doing so.    
             
Let me try to unpack this. Let's say that you're watching TV and you learn that the next thing coming on the station you're watching is a telethon meant to raise awareness of leukemia, and this will include kids who are suffering from leukemia telling their story. I bet you would predict, rightly, that watching this telethon would cause lots of empathy to bubble up within you. Do you stick to the channel and watch it, or do you turn away?                 

There are lots of motives you might have for watching. For one, you might be curious about the plight of folks living with leukemia. You might even feel that it's your moral responsibility to find out more about this group. You might also imagine that you'll be inspired to donate money to this cause, and that would make you feel good, as though you're living in accordance with your virtues and principles.                
There might also be reasons that you don't want to watch this telethon. For one, it might hurt. It would probably be heart wrenching to hear these stories. It might also make you experience guilt, especially if after watching this you choose not to donate. If you're strapped for cash, you might feel as though empathy will place you in a double bind, where you have to choose between your wallet on the one hand and your conscience on the other.               
  
Those might be situations you want to avoid, by avoiding empathy in the first place. I use the terms empathic approach motives and empathic avoidance motives to describe drives that push people towards and away from other people's emotions. People carry those motives out in lots of different ways. For instance, if I don't want to empathize with you, one strategy is I can just avoid you altogether. People often avoid situations that they think will inspire empathy in them. I can also simply not pay attention to your emotions, or decide through some appraisal process that your emotions are not important, or at least less important than my own.                 

Over the last couple of years, I've gathered evidence in support of a motivated view of empathy. For instance, when it comes to avoiding empathy, we can go to the example I just mentioned. You might be worried that empathy will cause you to feel guilty or morally obliged to part with some of your money. It might be a costly emotion.                 
It turns out that Dan Batson, about twenty years ago, ran a beautiful study in which he demonstrated this in a simple experiment. He told some people that they'd have a chance to donate to a homeless person, and he told other people that they'd have no such opportunity. He then asked people which of two appeals they wanted to hear: one objective story about this person's life, and another that was emotionally evocative. It turns out that people who thought they'd have a chance to donate tended to choose the emotionally neutral version of the story, consistent with the idea that they want to avoid experiencing empathy.                 

Another reason you might not want to experience empathy is if you're in the position where you have to harm somebody. Let's say that you're a linebacker, for instance, and you have to deliver a vicious tackle to a running back. It probably would behoove you to not feel everything that that person is feeling and think a lot about their emotions or the pain you're causing them. This happens in much darker contexts, of course.

In war, soldiers are explicitly encouraged to dehumanize their enemy, to make it less guilt inducing when they have to harm those people. This is what my colleague Al Bandura would call a moral disengagement. Al and his colleagues a few years ago demonstrated this in a very interesting and, to me, troubling way. They found that prison guards, especially executioners, tended to downplay the suffering of death row inmates consistent with their motive to do so and avoid guilt at their own actions. You see this all the time in modern warfare. Drone strikes, for instance, are a great way to avoid empathizing with the targets of an attack.             
    
Like I said, people are not just motivated to avoid empathy. People approach empathy as well. One example of this is loneliness. People who are lonely feel a deep desire to connect with others, and in many cases they do so by ramping up their empathy and focusing more on other people's minds and experiences.

Jon Maner and Adam Waytz and others have demonstrated that if you induce someone to be lonely, they'll pay more attention to other people's minds, connect more with their emotions. They'll even pay attention to minds that are not there. For instance, anthropomorphizing objects like robots. We see this to an extreme degree in the movie Cast Away, where Tom Hanks is so lonely that he anthropomorphizes and empathizes with a volleyball, Wilson, and thinks a lot about a mind that he imagines to be there but is actually not.               

Another reason you might want to empathize is when it's socially desirable to do so. If you learn that people around you value empathy, you might be encouraged to experience empathy yourself. One of my favorite studies on this works over the concept of gender roles in empathy. Thomas and Maio, seven or eight years ago, ran a study in which they started out by demonstrating that on a standard empathy test heterosexual men fared a little bit worse than women. This of course plays into the stereotype that women are more empathic than men, but the reason I like this study so much is because Thomas and Maio demonstrated that this is not a constitutional difference in the abilities of men, but probably instead represents a difference in their motivation. In a second study, these scientists convinced men that women find sensitive guys attractive, and it turns out that this motivation eliminated the gender gap in empathy performance. Straight man who believed that being empathic would make them attractive became more empathic, consistent with this motivated account under which people choose to approach or avoid empathy depending on their goals in a given situation.             

What is empathy, and where does it come from in our intellectual landscape? The term, empathy, [was] generated by German aesthetic philosophers. The term in German is Einfühlung, which is when you "feel yourself into" an art object. The term originates from Theodor Lipps, following Robert Vischer, another aesthetic philosopher. They both believed that the way we make contact with art is not by assessing its qualities in an objective sense, but by feeling into it, by projecting ourselves emotionally into a work. That was translated into English 106 years ago by Titchener, into the word empathy.

It's funny, if you use Google "engram," to examine the popularity of the term empathy, it looks like it's got a hydraulic relationship with another word: sympathy. Sympathy used to be much more popular. It has declined in popularity, and empathy has risen in popularity at the same time. It's a meaningful distinction between these two things. To my mind, sympathy is a more detached form of pity that you might have for someone suffering, whereas empathy requires a lot more emotional investment.                 

Empathy is expensive, psychologically. It costs a lot to empathize with someone, and there are many cases in which you might not want to do so. Scarcity is one thing that drives empathy down, stress is another. This is why they say that virtues are easier to abide on a full stomach. Empathy is, as well. If you are worried about survival and the well-being of yourself and your closest kin—your family—it's much harder to extend the "diameter" of your empathy to larger social groups.  Steve Pinker talks about this in The Better Angels of Our Nature. Peter Singer also talks about this in The Expanding Circle, the idea that maybe we can again expand the diameter of our concern for others, and maybe we have over the last decades. Even within one person's life, within a moment in time, there are many factors that might drive you to feel empathy or not.        

The costs of empathy include when it's painful, but also the responsibility that it places on people. If you empathize with someone, it's hard to compete with them. If you empathize with nonhuman animals, it's difficult to consume them. There is a moral responsibility that comes with an experience of empathy, especially if you want to continue being an emotionally authentic person.    
         
It does seem as though the social norms surrounding empathy have shifted. That's important, because if you view empathy not as a fixed quality of who we are—something that just happens automatically—but instead view it as something that we choose, then cultural landscapes should shape our individual emotional landscapes. We live in a more empathy-positive time than the past. People value warmth towards others and care for others as part of what it means to be a good person, now more than ever. That can make big changes in the way that people experience empathy.                 

Erik Nook and I along with our colleagues, ran a study recently where we saw whether conformity can generate empathy in people. If you believe that others around you value empathy, are you more likely to value it yourself? We found that people were. If we convinced folks that their peers experience lots of empathy, then our participants themselves reported more empathy and acted more kindly towards strangers, even if those strangers belonged to stigmatized outgroups. We think that a changing tide in our culture can change the way that people choose to engage with empathy.                 

Obama is probably the most empathy-focused President—or at least the President who uses the term the most—I’ve seen in my lifetime. He often talks about there being an empathy deficit, and says that one of the ways that we need to improve our society and the fabric of our society, is by increasing our empathy. I bet there are a lot of people who would disagree with that as a policy for running a state.  You saw this when Obama appointed Sotomayor as a Supreme Court justice and said this is a woman who has great empathy for the plight of many people. Well, that statement was vilified. He was pilloried for saying that, and people felt as though empathy is one of the worst features that you could select for when thinking about policy, when thinking about law, when thinking about government, because empathy is an emotion subject to all sorts of irrational biases. Justice should be blind, and presumably emotionally neutral.                 

You see this a lot in a movement that's taken hold recently. Paul Bloom and a set of other psychologists have made what I think is a great and very interesting case that empathy is overrated, especially as a moral compass. Their view is that empathy generates kind and moral behaviors, but in fundamentally skewed ways, for instance, only towards members of your own group and not in ways that maximize well-being across the largest number of people. On this account, empathy is an inflexible emotional engine for driving moral behavior and if you want to do the right thing, you should focus on more objective principles to guide your decision-making.      
           
That's a great argument. It's not one that I agree with. It follows from somewhat of an incomplete view of what empathy is. If you believe that empathy is automatic and either just happens to you or doesn't, then sure, the biases that characterize empathy are inescapable and will always govern empathic decision-making. If you instead view empathy as something that people can control, then people can choose to align their empathy more with their values.     
   
Empathy has a long tradition in lots of different fields. In philosophy, you've got Edith Stein, for instance, a nun who wrote beautifully about empathy. Also, Martin Buber—I and Thou is a beautiful book about how people connect with each other and share each other's experience.                 

Within psychology, the study of empathy has an equally long history, and one that's got a lot of players in it. For my money, the most powerful research on empathy in the 20th century comes from Dan Batson, who's for decades demonstrated the power of emotional connection to drive people to helping each other—thinking about empathy as an engine for promoting cooperation and altruism.  
              
The APA recently discovered that a set of psychologists had aided and abetted in a program of enhanced interrogation, hugely controversial and enormously problematic. It's so horrific to think about psychology being used in this way, but you can imagine how that works.                 

I mentioned earlier that individuals with psychopathy can understand what people feel, but they use that understanding not to improve other people's states, but sometimes to worsen their states. In a perverse way, the torturer needs to engage with at least some forms of empathy in order to do their job effectively. They need to know how to push someone's buttons, how to generate as much distress as they can. This is the dark side of empathy.

There is a dark side of empathy, especially when you experience one piece of empathy without the others. Understanding someone, having emotional intelligence, might just make you a better manipulator, if you're so inclined.        

Viewing empathy as a choice helps us understand the basic nature of empathy, why and when people empathize and why and when they don't. It is more powerful than that, because it can also help us address what Mina Cikara and I have called empathic failures, cases in which people don't empathize, and that generates some problems down the line.                 

I mentioned this already with respect to intergroup conflicts, but empathic failures happen in lots of other settings. For instance, when adolescents bully each other, or when physicians fail to understand the suffering of their patients, those are empathic failures. There are lots of interventionists doing hard and important work to try to mitigate the effects of empathic failures. This type of intervention tends to take on one of two flavors: either teaching people empathic skills, like how to recognize other people's emotions, or giving them opportunities to empathize, for instance, taking groups of people who are in conflict and having them spend time together.  
               
This is a great approach, but viewing empathy as a motivated phenomenon encourages us to take another approach as well, not just teaching people how to empathize, but getting them to want to empathize in the first place. Not just training skills, but also building motives.                 

That is what my lab has been up to for the last couple of years; we've been generating and testing social psychological “nudges” that might encourage people to want to empathize. We're excited to bring this into a bunch of spheres, including testing whether we can reduce bullying in adolescents and help physicians be more effective in treating their patients.        

There are huge disparities in how people feel about empathy and what they think it is, depending on where they fall on political and social landscapes. Both more conservative and more liberal people can be extremely empathic or very un-empathic. The question is, empathy for whom? Folks on the right end of the political spectrum tend to be more empathic with members of their group; they're oriented towards tradition, and towards establishing connections with people who are part of those traditions.    
    
At least the cultural mores progressives tend to be more indiscriminate and to value, in some egalitarian way, the emotions of everybody. There are shifting cultural norms surrounding who should be empathic and who should not be empathic. Men and women, for instance, are stereotyped into roles that drive them towards being more and less empathic, respectively. That is almost certainly a holdover from previous generations.

To my mind, these constructions of empathy, as a Republican or a Democratic thing or a male or a female thing, are historical more than they are embedded in the structure of who we are. One of the things that's curious to me is how empathy changes over time. A very famous and quite controversial study that came out a few years ago by Sara Konrath and her colleagues at Michigan, found that college students report being much less empathic now than they did thirty years ago. There's a drop-off in empathy that's pretty steady across that thirty-year period, but especially pronounced in the last ten years. People have jumped on the idea that this has to do with electronic forms of communication, people losing out on face-to-face contact in favor of contact that's mediated by some an electronic device.                 

That's an interesting assertion. To my mind, it's an easy conclusion to draw. I would be just as likely to believe that people are not necessarily more or less empathic, but rather, they feel that empathy is something different, and they might not be as drawn to empathy as a construct. They might not feel that it's as desirable as it was thirty years ago. This, of course, is interesting because thirty years ago is the middle of the '80s, which people probably don't consider the most empathic decade on record. Nonetheless, when we see changes in peoples' empathic experience across cultural lines, across time, across gender, it might reflect not only who people are, but who they want to be.        

My hope for our ongoing work and this line of thinking is that it can teach people about empathy, but also teach people how to work with their own empathy. This is one of those cases where education and intervention overlap. If you believe that you can harness empathy and make choices about when to experience it versus when not to, it adds a layer of responsibility to how you engage with other people. If you feel like you're powerless to control your empathy, you might be satisfied with whatever biases and limits you have on it. You might be okay with not caring about someone just because they're different from you. I want people to not feel safe empathizing in the way that they always have. I want them to understand that they're doing something deliberate when they connect with someone, and I want them to own that responsibility.      
  
It's easy to overdose on empathy, and empathy can be dangerous thing for an individual's well-being. It can cause you to burn out. There's something known as compassion fatigue, for hospice nurses and physicians working in hospices. These are people who overload themselves on other people's suffering to the point that they can't take care of themselves anymore.

The idea that you can control empathy is not just meant so that everyone can turn their empathy up to eleven all the time. It's just as important to know when to turn down one's empathy, especially if you need to engage in self-care. If you need to take care of yourself, sometimes it's important to not empathize.       
          
My wife is a clinical psychologist, and she says that the last thing that any of her patients need if they're depressed, is for her to be depressed as well. She needs to modulate her empathy on line in order to be able to guide those people towards something that will help them, not just showing them that she feels the same thing as them, but being a source of comfort for them. That requires knowing not just how to turn up empathy, but also how to turn it down sometimes.        

There are cases in which people can use other people's empathy to take advantage of them or manipulate them. Advertisers do this all the time, and politicians. People try to narrativize their ideas and turn them into stories about people's suffering so that you will feel more connected to them. Any ad for Save The Children starts with an example of a child who's in dire straights and the only way that this child will survive is if you help them. This is explicitly meant to tug on people's heartstrings in a very particular way. I don't think that empathy is necessarily always morally positive or negative, it's somewhat neutral and it's really in the way that you use it.

Selected and edited from - EchoDOTcom


** **

       Post - Amorella